|
|
Line 91: |
Line 91: |
|
| |
|
| </tabber> | | </tabber> |
| <restricted>
| |
|
| |
|
| == Useful Resources ==
| | {{#restricted:{{9.2 Biases}}}}{{NavCard|prev=9.1 Heuristics|next=10.1 Confirmation Bias}} |
| | |
| <tabber>
| |
| | |
| |-|Lecture Video=
| |
| | |
| <br /><center><youtube>_iKuqvhyE6A</youtube></center><br />
| |
| | |
| |-|Discussion Slides=
| |
| | |
| {{LinkCard | |
| |url=https://docs.google.com/presentation/d/11jcf9OQnHfYTuYYKijC6eLhGUe4q49lxiHmNKg-WxNk/
| |
| |title=Discussion Slides Template
| |
| |description=The discussion slides for this lesson.
| |
| }}
| |
| <br />
| |
| | |
| </tabber>
| |
| | |
| == Recommended Outline ==
| |
| | |
| === Before Class ===
| |
| | |
| {{BoxCaution|This class in particular has ''a lot'' of content. Make sure to take extra time when reviewing this lesson.}}
| |
| Prepare enough sticky notes for your class for the [[#Fundamental Attribution Error|fundamental attribution error]] activity.
| |
| | |
| === During Class ===
| |
| | |
| {| class="wikitable" style="margin-left: 0px; margin-right: auto;"
| |
| |21 Minutes
| |
| |Lead the students through the [[#Conformity|conformity]] discussion.
| |
| |-
| |
| |24 Minutes
| |
| |Lead the students through the [[#Obedience|obedience]] discussion.
| |
| |-
| |
| |11 Minutes
| |
| |Play the [[#Fundamental Attribution Error|fundamental attribution error]] game.
| |
| |-
| |
| |12 Minutes
| |
| |Lead the students through the [[#Status Quo Bias|status quo bias]] discussion. Skip this if pressed for time.
| |
| |-
| |
| |8 Minutes
| |
| |Lead the students through the [[#Temporal Discounting|temporal discounting]] discussion.
| |
| |}
| |
| | |
| == Lesson Content ==
| |
| | |
| === Conformity ===
| |
| | |
| ==== Instructions ====
| |
| | |
| {| class="wikitable" style="margin-left: 0px; margin-right: auto;"
| |
| |6 Minutes
| |
| |Show your students the [[#Asch Experiment|Asch experiment video]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |8 Minutes
| |
| |Have your students discuss the [[#Asch Question 1|first Asch discussion question]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |7 Minutes
| |
| |Have your students discuss the [[#Asch Question 2|second Asch discussion question]].
| |
| |}
| |
| | |
| ==== Asch Experiment ====
| |
| | |
| <center><youtube>NyDDyT1lDhA</youtube></center>
| |
| | |
| ==== Asch Question 1 ====
| |
| | |
| Why do you think the participants in the Asch experiment conformed, even though they could see the other people were not correct? Can you think of a time you conformed without really thinking about it? Why did you conform then? Can you think of a time you didn't conform? Why didn't you? Was it difficult? How did others react?
| |
| | |
| ==== Asch Question 2 ====
| |
| | |
| Discuss as a whole group. What surprised you? Are there sometimes good reasons to conform? What makes it hard to resist conforming? What enables people to resist conformity?
| |
| | |
| === Obedience ===
| |
| | |
| This activity introduces students to the unexpected power of the pressure to obey authority, showing them the classic Milgram study and inviting them to discuss its implications and ramifications.
| |
| | |
| ==== Instructions ====
| |
| | |
| {| class="wikitable" style="margin-left: 0px; margin-right: auto;"
| |
| |6 Minutes
| |
| |Show your students the [[#Milgram Experiment|Milgram experiment video]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |10 Minutes
| |
| |Have your students discuss the [[#Milgram Group Discussion|small group discussion questions]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |8 Minutes
| |
| |Have your students discuss the [[#Milgram Class Discussion|whole class discussion questions]].
| |
| |}
| |
| | |
| ==== Milgram Experiment ====
| |
| | |
| <center><youtube>wObjVgDHxD8</youtube></center>
| |
| {{BoxCaution|Warn students that the video may be disturbing, and assure them that no one was actually physically injured.}}
| |
| {{BoxTip|The full version of the video is hidden in the slides. You may show it if you'd like.}}
| |
| ==== Milgram Group Discussion ====
| |
| | |
| Have your students discuss the following questions in small groups.
| |
| # Why did so many people continue to push the button, even after the "learner" cried out in pain and begged them to stop?
| |
| # What situational factors do you think contributed to people feeling pressure to push the button?
| |
| # What situational factors might increase people's willingness to push the button? What situational factors might decrease their willingness? What questions do you have for plenary?
| |
| # What would prevent you from pushing the button all the way through? How can you be sure you wouldn't? (How similar would the situation have to be for you to recognize the similarity to Milgram's study?)
| |
| # What could we do at the level of institutions that might reduce the pressure to conform and obey to do harmful acts?
| |
| | |
| ==== Milgram Class Discussion ====
| |
| | |
| [[File:Milgram variations.gif|thumb|Variations on the Milgram experiment.]]
| |
| Return to the whole class. Explain that Milgram did many variations on this experiment, and found that the further away the experimenter, the less people pushed the button; and the closer the victim, the less people pushed the button. Ask your students the following question.
| |
| # Why do you think experimenter distance made it easier to disobey?
| |
| # Why do you think victim proximity made it easier to disobey?
| |
| # What implications does this study have for our society? For the increasingly technological nature of war (e.g. drones piloted at a distance)?
| |
| {{BoxCaution|You may want to give your students a couple minutes to get up and shake it off before continuing.}} | |
| === Fundamental Attribution Error ===
| |
| | |
| In this activity students are introduced to the broader phenomenon of the fundamental attribution error. The FAE describes people's surprise at both the Milgram and Asch experiments, as well as other situations where people underestimate the power of the situation. In other words, a lot of bad behavior comes not from bad people doing bad things, but from regular people in difficult situations. We can take this optimistically; it's not that people are intrinsically bad, it's that it's really hard to resist situations. So if we can change the situations—if we can make systemic change happen—then maybe we can drastically reduce harmful behavior. Students will practice generating alternative situational explanations to help them recognize the power of situation, and to reduce the natural tendency to underestimate it.
| |
| {{LinkCard
| |
| |title=Fundamental Attribution Error Game
| |
| |url=https://sites.google.com/a/sunsetparkhighschool.org/psychology/sociocultural/activity-fundamental-attribution-error
| |
| |description=The game that inspired this activity.}}
| |
| ==== Instructions ====
| |
| | |
| {| class="wikitable" style="margin-left: 0px; margin-right: auto;"
| |
| |7 Minutes
| |
| |Have your students [[#FAE Game Directions|play the game]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |4 Minutes
| |
| |Ask your students the [[#FAE Game Discussion|discussion questions]].
| |
| |}
| |
| | |
| ==== FAE Game Directions ====
| |
| | |
| {{BoxCaution|Make sure you bring enough sticky notes for your students.}}
| |
| Give the students an example and then the following directions.
| |
| # Write some strange past behavior of yours that could be misinterpreted on the sticky note.
| |
| # Once everyone in your group has written them, trade the notes with another group and try to come up with creative explanations for them.
| |
| # Once you have explanations for the other group, get back together and share your explanations and the real ones.
| |
| Afterwards, call the class back together and see if anyone has any particularly amusing past behaviors and misinterpretations to share.
| |
| | |
| ==== FAE Game Discussion ====
| |
| | |
| Ask your students the following questions.
| |
| # Why is it so easy to jump to conclusions?
| |
| # Why is it useful to consider multiple possible explanations for a behavior?
| |
| # How can we improve our institutions to reduce situational pressures that often cause harmful behavior (e.g. through obedience and conformity, as in the Milgram and Asch experiments)?
| |
| | |
| === Status Quo Bias ===
| |
| {{BoxTip|Skip this activity if you run out of time.}}
| |
| Students are introduced to status quo bias, and discuss its justifications, injustices, and ramifications for society.
| |
| | |
| ==== Instructions ====
| |
| | |
| {| class="wikitable" style="margin-left: 0px; margin-right: auto;"
| |
| |3 Minutes
| |
| |Briefly [[#Status Quo Bias Explanation|explain status quo bias]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |4 Minutes
| |
| |Have your students discuss the [[#Status Quo Bias Group Discussion|small group discussion questions]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |5 Minutes
| |
| |Have your students discuss the [[#Status Quo Bias Class Discussion|whole class discussion questions]].
| |
| |}
| |
| | |
| ==== Status Quo Bias Explanation ====
| |
| | |
| ''Status quo'' is Latin for "the state in which." It means the current way things are. The status quo bias is thus the tendency to want to keep things the way they are, an attitude of "if it ain't broke don't fix it" and sometimes "changing it will only make things worse" or "if you can't make it perfect why bother to change it at all."
| |
| | |
| ==== Status Quo Bias Group Discussion ====
| |
| | |
| Have your students discuss the following in small groups.
| |
| # What are some reasons that the status quo bias might be rational in some circumstances?
| |
| # What are some reasons that the status quo bias might be irrational in some circumstances?
| |
| # How do we distinguish what aspects of the status quo are worth maintaining, and which are worth changing?
| |
| | |
| ==== Status Quo Bias Class Discussion ====
| |
| | |
| Call the whole class back together and ask them what insights arose in the small group discussion.
| |
| {{BoxTip|Make sure someone mentions that status quo bias rests on the assumption that if we do nothing, things will remain stable. In our current situation of accelerating technological change, this is not true.}}
| |
| === Temporal Discounting ===
| |
| {{BoxTip|Skip this activity if you run out of time.}}
| |
| Students are introduced to temporal discounting and discuss its implications for personal and societal decision-making.
| |
| | |
| ==== Instructions ====
| |
| | |
| {| class="wikitable" style="margin-left: 0px; margin-right: auto;"
| |
| |4 Minutes
| |
| |Briefly [[#Temporal Discounting Explanation|explain temporal discounting]].
| |
| |-
| |
| |4 Minutes
| |
| |Ask your students the [[#Temporal Discounting Discussion Questions|discussion questions]]. Spend about a minute on each question.
| |
| |}
| |
| | |
| ==== Temporal Discounting Explanation ====
| |
| | |
| Tell the students that "temporal discounting" is our tendency to “discount” the value of goods/events in the future compared to ones in the present. Typically, we perceive the value as continuing to go down the farther in the future we go. For example, when asked if they'll take one cookie now or 3 cookies in 4 days people are more likely to take the cookie now.
| |
| {{BoxCaution|Temporal discounting is generally true for short time scales and small rewards. But, it gets more nuanced in other cases.}}
| |
| ==== Temporal Discounting Discussion Questions ====
| |
| | |
| <ol start=1><li>Why would we value the same amount of money more now than in the future?</li></ol>
| |
| {{BoxAnswer|Risk/uncertainty/distrust of promiser, inflation, shortsightedness.}}
| |
| <ol start=2><li>Temporal discounting preferences are subjective. Under what conditions, if any, can we say they are irrational?</li></ol>
| |
| {{BoxAnswer|If we can know with high confidence that the prize would be more valuable to our future self.}}
| |
| <ol start=3><li>How might the tendency for steep temporal discounting play out in policymaking?</li></ol>
| |
| {{BoxAnswer|It may lead to undervaluing consequences for our future selves, and even more so future people who may not be born yet, e.g. in polluting the environment for immediate economic gain at the cost of longer term costs, both economic and otherwise.}}
| |
| <ol start=4><li>How does the tendency to value present goods over future equal goods exacerbate climate change?</li></ol>
| |
| {{BoxAnswer|It makes us overvalue current gains in economy or convenience at the expense of long-term consequences for our future selves and future people.}}
| |
| <ol start=5><li>'''Extension/Stretch/Bonus:''' Because of the hyperbolic shape of the temporal discounting curve, it's easier to give up soon-but-future rewards for much later and larger rewards than immediate-but-smaller rewards. This is one possible explanation for the movie-choosing pattern:<ol>
| |
| <li>When told to choose a movie to watch tonight vs. next week, people are more likely to choose an action flick or a chick flick for tonight, and more likely to choose a documentary or something more artistic for next week. Why do you think this is?</li>
| |
| <li>Given this pattern, how does the transition from ordering, borrowing, or buying videos to watching everything streaming online change our video-watching habits? Is there any way to counteract this?</li>
| |
| </ol></ol></restricted>{{NavCard|prev=9.1 Heuristics|next=10.1 Confirmation Bias}}
| |
| [[Category:Lesson plans]] | | [[Category:Lesson plans]] |